Babies are smart - give them the love and respect they deserve
http://www.cnn.com/2011/10/23/opinion/gopnik-ted-children-learning/index.html
Babies are smarter than you think
2:57 PM EST, Sun October 23, 2011
Editor's note: Alison Gopnik is
a professor of psychology and philosophy at the University of California at
Berkeley. Her books include "The Philosophical Baby" and "The
Scientist in the Crib." TED is a nonprofit dedicated to "Ideas worth
spreading," which it makes available through talks posted on its website.
(CNN) -- In the past 30 years we've learned that babies and
young children know more and learn more than we would ever have thought
possible.
Philosophers and psychologists, even the great Swiss child-development
theorist Jean Piaget, once thought that babies and young children were
irrational, solipsistic, illogical and amoral -- unable to take the perspective
of others or understand cause and effect. But new scientific techniques have
taught us that even the youngest infants already know a great deal about
objects, people and language, and learn even more. In fact, they have implicit
learning methods that are as powerful and intelligent as those of the smartest
scientists.
They can unconsciously do complicated statistical analyses and their everyday
play turns out, remarkably, to be very much like a set of scientific
experiments. And I, at least, think that they may actually experience the world
more vividly than we do.
Here's just one example of this new research:
One of the hardest problems for all of us is figuring out what other people
want, think and feel. It's especially difficult when what they want is different
from what we want ourselves. Traditionally, psychologists thought that children
couldn't take the perspective of other people until they were 8 or so. But my
student Betty Repacholi and I gave 15- and 18-month-olds two bowls of food, one
of raw broccoli and one of goldfish crackers.
The children, even in Berkeley, liked the crackers and didn't like the
broccoli. With the children watching, Betty tasted a little food from each bowl
and made either a disgusted face or a happy face. Then she again gave the babies
both bowls of food, put out her hand and said, "Can you give me some?"
The 18-month-olds, just barely walking and talking, gave her the crackers if she
had acted as if she liked the crackers and the broccoli if she had acted as if
she liked the broccoli.
These very young children had the profound understanding that someone else --
Betty, in this case -- might have a different perspective on the world, or at
least on broccoli, and they helped her get what she wanted. The 15-month-olds,
on the other hand, only gave her the crackers. This suggested something even
more remarkable: Babies somehow learned this deep fact about human nature
between 15 and 18 months. Other studies have shown that this kind of learning is
the result of statistical analyses and the everyday experimentation we call
play.
This work was inspired by purely scientific, and even philosophical,
questions. How can we human beings ever learn as much as we do from the few
photons hitting our retinas and the air disturbances that hit our eardrums? How
is it that the few genes that separate us from chimpanzees could lead to such
big differences in the way we think and live? How can we ever know what someone
else thinks or feels?
It turns out, surprisingly enough, that studying babies and young children
can hold answers to those big questions. In fact, from an evolutionary point of
view it seems that our exceptionally long childhood may play a crucial role in
many of the abilities that make us distinctly human.
But this basic science also has implications for what we do. Science has
demonstrated just how crucially important the very early years are. And yet more
than 20% of American children still grow up in poverty, and preschool teachers
are paid less than dogcatchers. We have fewer programs of parental leave or
subsidies for childcare than exist in almost every other civilized country.
The programs we do have to help support and encourage early childhood
learning, like Head Start, are facing cuts, even though studies show that in the
long run they have the best payoffs of any public investment. The scientific
work on the way babies learn demonstrates that neglecting our youngest children
is self-destructive. Our moral intuition ought to tell us it's just plain wrong.
On the other hand, when parents, or even policy-makers hear about how much
babies learn, they often conclude that what we need to do is teach them more.
Parents spend literally millions of dollars on "educational" toys,
videos and programs, that they hope will somehow give their children an edge.
Parents and policy-makers pressure teachers to make preschools more and more
academic, with more reading drills and less time for play and pretend. But the
science suggests this is also wrong. Very young children learn best from their
everyday experiences of people and things, and from being able to playfully
explore the world in a safe setting with people who love and care for them.
Those settings can't be mass manufactured or provided on the cheap, and the
learning they lead to can't be simply measured on standardized tests.
The science of early childhood is constantly surprising -- who would guess
that 2-year-olds can use statistics to test hypotheses? But actually the policy
implications fit what most preschool teachers know intuitively: Children thrive
when they are loved, and they learn when they explore. The real mystery is why
we can't get the politicians to see it, too.